miércoles, 30 de mayo de 2012

ANTONIO DAMÁSIO: The quest to understand consciousness.

http://www.ted.com/talks/lang/en/antonio_damasio_the_quest_to_understand_consciousness.html

I'm here to talk about the wonder and the mystery of conscious minds. The wonder is about the fact that we all woke up this morning and we had with it the amazing return of our conscious mind. We recovered minds with a complete sense of self and a complete sense of our own existence, yet we hardly ever pause to consider this wonder. We should, in fact, because without having this possibility of conscious minds, we would have no knowledge whatsoever about our humanity; we would have no knowledge whatsoever about the world. We would have no pains, but also no joys. We would have no access to love or to the ability to create. And of course, Scott Fitzgerald said famously that "he who invented consciousness would have a lot to be blamed for." But he also forgot that without consciousness, he would have no access to true happiness and even the possibility of transcendence.
So much for the wonder, now for the mystery. This is a mystery that has really been extremely hard to elucidate. All the way back into early philosophy and certainly throughout the history of neuroscience, this has been one mystery that has always resisted elucidation, has got major controversies. And there are actually many people that think we should not even touch it; we should just leave it alone, it's not to be solved. I don't believe that, and I think the situation is changing. It would be ridiculous to claim that we know how we make consciousness in our brains, but we certainly can begin to approach the question, and we can begin to see the shape of a solution.
And one more wonder to celebrate is the fact that we have imaging technologies that now allow us to go inside the human brain and be able to do, for example, what you're seeing right now. These are images that come from Hanna Damasio's lab, and which show you, in a living brain, the reconstruction of that brain. And this is a person who is alive. This is not a person that is being studied at autopsy. And even more -- and this is something that one can be really amazed about -- is what I'm going to show you next, which is going underneath the surface of the brain and actually looking in the living brain at real connections, real pathways. So all of those colored lines correspond to bunches of axons, the fibers that join cell bodies to synapses. And I'm sorry to disappoint you, they don't come in color. But at any rate, they are there. The colors are codes for the direction, from whether it is back to front or vice versa.
At any rate, what is consciousness? What is a conscious mind? And we could take a very simple view and say, well, it is that which we lose when we fall into deep sleep without dreams, or when we go under anesthesia, and it is what we regain when we recover from sleep or from anesthesia. But what is exactly that stuff that we lose under anesthesia, or when we are in deep, dreamless sleep? Well first of all, it is a mind, which is a flow of mental images. And of course consider images that can be sensory patterns, visual, such as you're having right now in relation to the stage and me, or auditory images, as you are having now in relation to my words. That flow of mental images is mind.
But there is something else that we are all experiencing in this room. We are not passive exhibitors of visual or auditory or tactile images. We have selves. We have a Me that is automatically present in our minds right now. We own our minds. And we have a sense that it's everyone of us that is experiencing this -- not the person who is sitting next to you. So in order to have a conscious mind, you have a self within the conscious mind. So a conscious mind is a mind with a self in it. The self introduces the subjective perspective in the mind, and we are only fully conscious when self comes to mind. So what we need to know to even address this mystery is, number one, how are minds are put together in the brain, and, number two, how selves are constructed.
Now the first part, the first problem, is relatively easy -- it's not easy at all -- but it is something that has been approached gradually in neuroscience. And it's quite clear that, in order to make minds, we need to construct neural maps. So imagine a grid, like the one I'm showing you right now, and now imagine, within that grid, that two-dimensional sheet, imagine neurons. And picture, if you will, a billboard, a digital billboard, where you have elements that can be either lit or not. And depending on how you create the pattern of lighting or not lighting, the digital elements, or, for that matter, the neurons in the sheet, you're going to be able to construct a map. This, of course, is a visual map that I'm showing you, but this applies to any kind of map -- auditory, for example, in relation to sound frequencies, or to the maps that we construct with our skin in relation to an object that we palpate.
Now to bring home the point of how close it is -- the relationship between the grid of neurons and the topographical arrangement of the activity of the neurons and our mental experience -- I'm going to tell you a personal story. So if I cover my left eye -- I'm talking about me personally, not all of you -- if I cover my left eye, I look at the grid -- pretty much like the one I'm showing you. Everything is nice and fine and perpendicular. But sometime ago, I discovered that if I cover my left eye, instead what I get is this. I look at the grid and I see a warping at the edge of my central-left field.
Very odd -- I've analyzed this for a while. But sometime ago, through the help of an opthamologist colleague of mine, Carmen Puliafito, who developed a laser scanner of the retina, I found out the the following. If I scan my retina through the horizontal plane that you see there in the little corner, what I get is the following. On the right side, my retina is perfectly symmetrical. You see the going down towards the fovea where the optic nerve begins. But on my left retina there is a bump, which is marked there by the red arrow. And it corresponds to a little cyst that is located below. And that is exactly what causes the warping of my visual image.
So just think of this: you have a grid of neurons, and now you have a plane mechanical change in the position of the grid, and you get a warping of your mental experience. So this is how close your mental experience and the activity of the neurons in the retina, which is a part of the brain located in the eyeball, or, for that matter, a sheet of visual cortex. So from the retina you go onto visual cortex. And of course, the brain adds on a lot of information to what is going on in the signals that come from the retina. And in that image there, you see a variety of islands of what I call image-making regions in the brain. You have the green for example, that corresponds to tactile information, or the blue that corresponds to auditory information.
And something else that happens is that those image-making regions where you have the plotting of all these neural maps, can then provide signals to this ocean of purple that you see around, which is the association cortex, where you can make records of what went on in those islands of image-making. And the great beauty is that you can then go from memory, out of those association cortices, and produce back images in the very same regions that have perception. So think about how wonderfully convenient and lazy the brain is. So it provides certain areas for perception and image-making. And those are exactly the same that are going to be used for image-making when we recall information.
So far the mystery of the conscious mind is diminishing a little bit because we have a general sense of how we make these images. But what about the self? The self is really the elusive problem. And for a long time, people did not even want to touch it, because they'd say, "How can you have this reference point, this stability, that is required to maintain the continuity of selves day after day?" And I thought about a solution to this problem. It's the following. We generate brain maps of the body's interior and use them as the reference for all other maps.
So let me tell you just a little bit about how I came to this. I came to this because, if you're going to have a reference that we know as self -- the Me, the I in our own processing -- we need to have something that is stable, something that does not deviate much from day to day. Well it so happens that we have a singular body. We have one body, not two, not three. And so that is a beginning. There is just one reference point, which is the body. But then, of course, the body has many parts, and things grow at different rates, and they have different sizes and different people; however, not so with the interior. The things that have to do with what is known as our internal milieu -- for example, the whole management of the chemistries within our body are, in fact, extremely maintained day after day for one very good reason. If you deviate too much in the parameters that are close to the midline of that life-permitting survival range, you go into disease or death. So we have an in-built system within our own lives that ensures some kind of continuity. I like to call it an almost infinite sameness from day to day. Because if you don't have that sameness, physiologically, you're going to be sick or you're going to die. So that's one more element for this continuity.
And the final thing is that there is a very tight coupling between the regulation of our body within the brain and the body itself, unlike any other coupling. So for example, I'm making images of you, but there's no physiological bond between the images I have of you as an audience and my brain. However, there is a close, permanently maintained bond between the body regulating parts of my brain and my own body.
So here's how it looks. Look at the region there. There is the brain stem in between the cerebral cortex and the spinal cord. And it is within that region that I'm going to highlight now that we have this housing of all the life-regulation devices of the body. This is so specific that, for example, if you look at the part that is covered in red in the upper part of the brain stem, if you damage that as a result of a stroke, for example, what you get is coma or vegetative state, which is a state, of course, in which your mind disappears, your consciousness disappears. What happens then actually is that you lose the grounding of the self, you have no longer access to any feeling of your own existence, and, in fact, there can be images going on, being formed in the cerebral cortex, except you don't know they're there. You have, in effect, lost consciousness when you have damage to that red section of the brain stem.
But if you consider the green part of the brain stem, nothing like that happens. It is that specific. So in that green component of the brain stem, if you damage it, and often it happens, what you get is complete paralysis, but your conscious mind is maintained. You feel, you know, you have a fully conscious mind that you can report very indirectly. This is a horrific condition. You don't want to see it. And people are, in fact, imprisoned within their own bodies, but they do have a mind. There was a very interesting film, one of the rare good films done about a situation like this, by Julian Schnabel some years ago about a patient that was in that condition.
So now I'm going to show you a picture. I promise not to say anything about this, except this is to frighten you. It's just to tell you that in that red section of the brain stem, there are, to make it simple, all those little squares that correspond to modules that actually make brain maps of different aspects of our interior, different aspects of our body. They are exquisitely topographic and they are exquisitely interconnected in a recursive pattern. And it is out of this and out of this tight coupling between the brain stem and the body that I believe -- and I could be wrong, but I don't think I am -- that you generate this mapping of the body that provides the grounding for the self and that comes in the form of feelings -- primordial feelings, by the way.
So what is the picture that we get here? Look at "cerebral cortex," look at "brain stem," look at "body," and you get the picture of the interconnectivity in which you have the brain stem providing the grounding for the self in a very tight interconnection with the body. And you have the cerebral cortex providing the great spectacle of our minds with the profusion of images that are, in fact, the contents of our minds and that we normally pay most attention to, as we should, because that's really the film that is rolling in our minds. But look at the arrows. They're not there for looks. They're there because there's this very close interaction. You cannot have a conscious mind if you don't have the interaction between cerebral cortex and brain stem. You cannot have a conscious mind if you don't have the interaction between the brain stem and the body.
Another thing that is interesting is that the brain stem that we have is shared with a variety of other species. So throughout vertebrates, the design of the brain stem is very similar to ours, which is one of the reasons why I think those other species have conscious minds like we do. Except that they're not as rich as ours, because they don't have a cerebral cortex like we do. That's where the difference is. And I strongly disagree with the idea that consciousness should be considered as the great product of the cerebral cortex. Only the wealth of our minds is, not the very fact that we have a self that we can refer to our own existence, and that we have any sense of person.
Now there are three levels of self to consider -- the proto, the core and the autobiographical. The first two are shared with many, many other species, and they are really coming out largely of the brain stem and whatever there is of cortex in those species. It's the autobiographical self which some species have, I think. Cetaceans and primates have also an autobiographical self to a certain degree. And everybody's dogs at home have an autobiographical self to a certain degree. But the novelty is here.
The autobiographical self is built on the basis of past memories and memories of the plans that we have made; it's the lived past and the anticipated future. And the autobiographical self has prompted extended memory, reasoning, imagination, creativity and language. And out of that came the instruments of culture -- religions, justice, trade, the arts, science, technology. And it is within that culture that we really can get -- and this is the novelty -- something that is not entirely set by our biology. It is developed in the cultures. It developed in collectives of human beings. And this is, of course, the culture where we have developed something that I like to call socio-cultural regulation.
And finally, you could rightly ask, why care about this? Why care if it is the brain stem or the cerebral cortex and how this is made? Three reasons. First, curiosity. Primates are extremely curious -- and humans most of all. And if we are interested, for example, in the fact that anti-gravity is pulling galaxies away from the Earth, why should we not be interested in what is going on inside of human beings?
Second, understanding society and culture. We should look at how society and culture in this socio-cultural regulation are a work in progress. And finally, medicine. Let's not forget that some of the worst diseases of humankind are diseases such as depression, Alzheimer's disease, drug addiction. Think of strokes that can devastate your mind or render you unconscious. You have no prayer of treating those diseases effectively and in a non-serendipitous way if you do not know how this works. So that's a very good reason beyond curiosity to justify what we're doing, and to justify having some interest in what is going on in our brains.
Thank you for your attention.
(Applause)

martes, 29 de mayo de 2012

FRASE DEL DÍA.

Mucha gente prefiere morrir antes que pensar. Y de hecho lo consiguen.
BERTRAND RUSSELL

lunes, 28 de mayo de 2012

Dr. Francesc Casadesús nuevo presidente de la SIFG.

DERECHO, FILOSOFÍA Y PSICOLOGÍA, FELICITA AL  NUEVO PRESIDENTE DE LA SIFG, EL DR. F. CASADESUS.


¨El doctor Francesc Casadesús, profesor del Departamento de Filosofía y Trabajo Social de la Universidad de las Islas Baleares (UIB), ha sido elegido nuevo presidente de la Sociedad Ibérica de Filosofía Griega (SIFG). La elección de la nueva junta directiva de la SIFG se produjo el pasado viernes, 25 de mayo, en la Universidad de Évora, en Portugal.
La SIFG, fundada en 1999 en la Facultad de Filosofía de la Universidad Complutense de Madrid, reúne los profesores, estudiosos y especialistas en filosofía griega de España y Portugal. Tiene el objetivo de fomentar el estudio, la investigación y la difusión de la filosofía griega, con especial atención a su proyección a lo largo de la tradición filosófica, y con la voluntad de difundir sus actividades en el ámbito internacional, preferentemente europeo e iberoamericano.
El doctor Francesc Casadesús es licenciado en Ciencias de la Información, en Filosofía y en Filología Clásica, y doctor en Filosofía. Actualmente es profesor titular de Filosofía Griega del Departamento de Filosofía y Trabajo Social de la UIB. Sus líneas de investigación se enmarcan en la filosofía presocrática, platónica, helenística y en la formación del vocabulario filosófico griego, así como en los estudios de las religiones mistéricas, como el orfismo y el dionisisme. Ha participado en numerosos congresos internacionales y ha publicado en libros y revistas. Es presidente de la Sección Balear de la Sociedad Española de Estudios Clásicos y coordinador de los XVI Cursos de Pensamiento y Cultura Clásica en Palma, XI en Menorca y VII en Ibiza, así como de las diez ediciones del Aula de Humanidades.¨
uib.es

jueves, 17 de mayo de 2012

SEMINARIO:LOS PROCESOS MENTALES Y NEURONALES LIGADOS A LA EXPERIENCIA ESTÉTICA

 NUEVO
 
SEMINARIO:  
LOS PROCESOS MENTALES Y NEURONALES LIGADOS A LA EXPERIENCIA ESTÉTICA
 
 LUGAR : Facultad de Psicología, Universidad Islas Baleares.
 
Miércoles 23 de mayo, a las 16:00 hs (lugar por determinar): Mesa redonda sobre neuroestética. 

Moderación: Marcos Nadal
Ponentes:  Martin Skov (Copenhagen Business School)
Anjan Chatterjee (University of Pennsylvania)

Jueves 24 de mayo, a las 10:00 (lugar por determinar): Reunión de profesores doctorandos con Martin Skov y Anjan Chatterjee para discutir tesis o proyectos de investigación.

Viernes 25 de mayo, a las 16:30 hs (lugar por determinar): Mesa redonda sobre psicología de la experiencia estética

Moderación: Marcos Nadal
Ponentes:  Helmut Leder (University of Vienna)
Pablo Tinio (New York University)

viernes, 4 de mayo de 2012

LA CIUDAD DE LAS IDEAS



FALTAN 188 DÍAS

Festival CDI 2012 El 8, 9 y 10 de noviembre


Poder Cívico y Puebla: La Ciudad de las Ideas

Una celebración de la creatividad.

Poder Cívico es una organización ciudadana, sin fines de lucro, apartidista y democrática. Creemos en el poder que las ideas tienen para cambiar actitudes, vidas y, ultimadamente, nuestra sociedad. En La Ciudad de las Ideas celebramos la creatividad y curiosidad del ser humano reuniendo a las mentes más provocadoras del mundo para que en sólo 21 minutos propongan nuevos paradigmas y fortalezcan a los ideastas (ciudadanos de La Ciudad de las Ideas) para elevar su potencial creativo.

Un festival que inspira

Cada año nos reunimos en Puebla, México, para explorar las ideas visionarias de los oradores más fascinantes de la actualidad. Desde su inicio, La Ciudad de las Ideas ha buscado interrogar ortodoxias y debatir los desafíos que enfrenta nuestra sociedad. Hacemos hincapié en la interacción y el acercamiento entre nuestros invitados destacados y todos los ideastas intelectualmente curiosos, con la valentía de pensar críticamente. Queremos que estas voces inspiradoras despierten ideas dentro de ti que ayuden abordar los problemas más urgentes de este planeta.

La ciudad de las ideas.com

INTELIGENCIA MÚLTIPLE ( 8 inteligencias)

http://youtu.be/xJkPGSJLuAU

jueves, 3 de mayo de 2012

LIBRO RECOMENDADO :INTELIGENCIAS MÚLTIPLES


SINOPSIS

Sin duda alguna, la teoría de las inteligencias múltiples de Howard Gardner ha revolucionado el mundo de la educación y de la psicología en las postrimerías del siglo XX. En efecto, la sugerente idea de que existen distintas capacidades humanas independientes, desde la inteligencia artificial hasta la que supone el conocimiento de uno mismo, ha atraído ya a innumerables educadores, padres e investigadores interesados por el papel del individuo en este proceso. 
El presente libro ofrece una especie de síntesis de toda esta labor con el objetivo de que resulte accesible para todo tipo de lectores. Se trata de un sugerente resumen de lo que ya se conoce como IM. En otras palabras, una propuesta moderna y arriesgada que ya representa toda una revolución, pues su meta es la búsqueda de informaciones directas acerca del modo en que la gente desarrolla capacidades que luego resultan ser muy importantes para su modo de vida: la inteligencia lingüística, la lógico-matemática, la espacial, la musical, la corporal y cinética, la interpersonal y la intrapersonal. Todas ellas, claro está, entendidas como potenciales biológicos en bruto que casi nunca pueden observarse en forma pura y que suelen trabajar siepre juntos para resolver problemas y alcanzar diversos fines culturales: vocaciones, aficiones y similares. 
La conclusión no es sólo que la escuela del futuro debería centrarse en el individuo y ayudarle a desarrollar todas sus inteligencias, sino también que cada persona tiene su propia manera de combinarlas y utilizarlas, aunque quizá no la conozca aún. En cualquier caso, la misión de este apasionante libro es lograr que la descubra.
Casa del libro

Prof. Dr.Howard Gardner


Howard Gardner and Multiple Intelligences

Howard Gardner is a Professor of Cognition and Education at Harvard University and Co-Director of Harvard's Project Zero. He is widely known for his Theory of Multiple Intelligences, introduced in his book Frames of Mind (1983). In his book, Gardner proposed a novel notion: the psychological construct 'intelligence' should be formally measured in more ways than simply through the widely accepted logical/linguistic IQ-type formalized tests used in most school systems. Frames of Mind was very well received by those in the educational arena.
Gardner suggests that our intelligences are organized vertically, as a number of almost different faculties, rather than horizontally, as a set of general abilities. This viewpoint was in direct contrast to many of the language and logic theorists who believe that there was only one kind of intelligence, that we either had a lot of it or not that much, and that there was virtually very little that we could be do about that.
In Frames, Gardner theorized eight basic intelligences to represent these other modes:
  • linguistic-verbal (most widely accepted)
  • logical-mathematical (most widely accepted)
  • visual-spatial
  • bodily-kinesthetic
  • musical-rhythmic
  • interpersonal (most criticized)
  • intrapersonal (most criticized)
  • naturalist (recently added)
nwlink.com

martes, 1 de mayo de 2012

LIBRO RECOMENDADO: EL ERROR DE DESCARTES


Sinopsis

¿Cuál es el error de Descartes? Para Antonio Damasio, uno de los más prestigiosos investigadores en neurofisiología, el de separar el cuerpo de la mente, con su tesis de que pensar es igual a ser, cuando se trata justamente de lo contrario: en el principio fue el ser, posteriormente el pensar; somos, luego pensamos.

Creer que las operaciones más refinadas de la mente están separadas de la estructura y del funcionamiento del organismo biológico es un error, porque el cerebro y el resto del cuerpo constituyen un organismo indisociable integrado por circuitos reguladores bioquímicos y neurales que se relacionan con el ambiente como un conjunto, y la actividad mental surge de esta interacción.

Esta innovadora visión del hombre se desarrolla en un libro que es, a la vez, riguroso y accesible. Partiendo de casos reales y bien documentados, el autor nos lleva a comprender cómo se forman las imágenes que percibimos, cómo se depositan nuestros conocimientos, cómo opera la memoria, cómo actúan los mecanismos reguladores de nuestra vida, qué son las emociones y sentimientos y, en definitiva, nos proporciona los conocimientos esenciales sobre el funcionamiento del cerebro.